See this doc for the previous version of this post which goes with the second post here. Some things in those are wrong.
This version of this post is going to be a bit messy. That’s because I’m not sure whether one of the L6 CT types is an essential component in all regions of all species, with at most limited exceptions. Also, some sources in this batch of notes are interesting but ambiguous, so overall I don’t have much of a framework for interpretation. I decided to just write what I think without getting into my reasoning too much, since that was taking forever to write and organize and it’s tentative anyway until I resolve the ambiguity.
I’ll add in text citations soon. The new sources start on page 20 in my notes.
I still need to check these facts since I wrote this mostly from memory.
The Thalamus
Thalamic cells have tonic mode and burst mode. In tonic mode, the cell fires normally. While inhibited, the cell is in burst mode. It will respond to the start of excitation with a brief additional excitation. This can cause a burst of spikes, but it can also cause a single spike because the size of the brief excitation depends on the duration and magnitude of the preceding inhibition. Bursting is related to oscillations because those provide periods of reduced excitation. Also, single burst mode spikes whether rhythmic or not send a larger signal than tonic mode spikes because of synaptic depression, which also makes later spikes in bursts less impactful.
The TRN receives excitation from and sends inhibition to TC cells. TRN and TC cells both have tonic and burst mode, and TRN cells are connected, so the TRN complicates the thalamus.
In primary thalamus and higher order thalamus respectively, the sensory input and layer 5 drive firing. L6 CT cells send metabotropic long lasting facilitating excitatory signals to all targets including the thalamus, which are fit to switch thalamic cells to tonic mode without exciting them suddenly enough to cause a burst. They target TC cells and TRN cells, so the signals are likely kept in check by inhibitory signals. Modulating TRN cells does not mean L6 CT cells can indirectly cause bursts, as tonic mode TRN cells probably only send metabotropic long lasting inhibitory signals when bursting, and otherwise only send normal inhibitory signals.
Bursting might serve to switch attention. If that is correct, then pathways through the thalamus are non-categorical and direct pathways are categorical because bursting operates in terms of the sensory input rather than the cortical representation, because the sensory input to O1 bypasses the thalamus, because of the parallel CC and CTC pathways up the cortical hierarchy, and because thalamic cells are more bursty in modalities and submodalities which require more changes in the attended sensory patches and less changes in the attended sensory items.
CT Cells and Cortex
The signals sent within the cortex are similar to those sent to the thalamus. The signals to excitatory cells facilitate, but they also target interneurons in each targeted layer. In L6 itself, they mainly target interneurons, although they could target excitatory L6 cells distally since those synapses usually go unnoticed using the methods of these studies. At least in L6, signals to excitatory cells always facilitate, but signals to most inhibitory cells facilitate once and then depress after the second spike. After a while, the signals to L6 excitatory cells might go out of control, so most likely those synapses are distal or interneurons in other layers are involved. CT cells slowly adapt, which is another way to keep facilitating signals in check.
Primary CT Cells
Primary CT cells only project to hierarchically lower thalamic nuclei. For example, in primary cortex, they only project to the primary thalamic nucleus, specifically the one for the same modality.
They receive input from the thalamus, but these synapses are weak and cannot cause firing alone. Instead, they are probably driven by L6 CC cells and possibly L5, both of which send horizontally wide-reaching axons which generally extend to the adjacent cortical column at least. Besides their driving inputs, primary CT cells are columnar. They probably only project to the patch of the thalamus which corresponds to the same column, and their axons and apical tufts are mostly in L4 of the same column. In barrel cortex, they are denser in columns than in the spaces between the columns. However, their columnar nature might not be universal because the second potentially non-universal system has a multi-columnar nature.
There are also dual-projecting CT cells and higher order CT cells in rodents, but these classes might be part of a separate system which might not be part of the fundamental circuitry. Higher order CT cells in these studies are only in the deepest fifth of L6 (L6Bb), which is not studied much so I will mostly ignore them until I find more information, but they are clearly a class in rodents. Dual-projecting CT cells are only in the lowest half of L6 (L6Ba and L6Bb), give or take a few percent, while primary CT cells are only in L6A and L6Ba.
The projections to L4 from primary CT cells probably activate excitatory cells. A study claimed that CT cells inhibit L4 more than they excite it, but based on the figures they accidentally only labelled and activated dual-projecting CT cells and maybe higher order CT cells. Pixel measurements confirm this problem, and the projections to L4 in barrel cortex are probably to the septal parts between columns.
CC Cells
Identifying CC cell types is difficult. They have several unusual morphological classes (like stellate cells and upside-down pyramidal cells, as well as normal pyramidal cells), but I do not have enough information about their inputs and outputs to tell whether they are functionally distinct. Besides CC and CT cells, there are also corticoclaustral cells, but those share many similarities with CC cells and I still need to research them. Lastly, there are Meynert cells in upper layer 6 of V1 which project both corticocortically and to the superior colliculus, but I have not heard of them outside V1.
At least in L6A, CC cells receive stronger inputs from the thalamus than CT cells, at least in terms of average experimentally noticed response. The inputs from primary and higher order thalamus are both larger, but especially the input from higher order thalamus. That does not mean it is a strong source of input, and the higher order nucleus in that study has some unusual or unique traits (the convergence zone discussed regarding the second system) so this input might not be universal.
CC cells respond to synaptic excitation and current injection with a brief depolarization which causes a single spike and then no firing, or briefly elevated firing and then either no firing or normal firing. I do not know whether there is a brief pause in firing immediately after the small number of more rapid spikes, which would make it a burst in a strict sense, but either way, other cells usually require apical input to burst (the exceptions are attributable to methods since they are so variable between studies) so this phasic firing is unusual if it occurs in normal function. Like TC cells, they fire more rapidly when they start firing, and their synapses depress.
Unlike the thalamus, however, CC cells can receive driving signals from hierarchically higher cortical regions. Specifically, M1-projecting CC cells (and perhaps others) receive strong motor copy signals. These M1-projecting cells are throughout L6 but denser in L6A, so they might be a subtype. Regardless, more than half of CC and CT cells respond to head rotation signals sent at least by retrosplenial cortex, so the CC cells probably receive driving motor copy signals. CC and CT cells are inhibited or excited as a result, with amplitude and signs depending on head rotation direction and speed. They could hypothetically only receive signals about rotation from motor cortex, but that seems unlikely because the study about the head rotation signals was on V1, for which other forms of motor copy signals (such as those which activate central pattern generators instead of just causing a joint to rotate) would be hard to find unlike in somatosensory cortex. However, it is also possible that M1 projects mainly to CC cells whereas the retrosplenial cortex projects to both CC and CT cells. Either way, CC and CT cells are both influenced by behavior.
This association with behavior might just reflect the second system since CC cells in upper L6 are denser in the spaces between columns (the septa) and the M1-projecting CC cells are denser in upper L6. That inter-column space is involved in the second system and it might only be activated during behavior by the thalamus, as well as by the direct motor copy signals described here.
CC cells of most or all types have transcolumnar axons in L6 which at least in the same column synapse on CT cells, CC cells, and inhibitory cells. These axons also form synapses in L5. I do not know whether the axons to or from L5 are mostly on interneurons like the axons from L2/3 to L4, although dual-projecting CT cells activate L5 slender tufted cells.
The Second System
The barrel cortex is weird. That is the impetus for this proposal, but it seems likely that the second system is universal to all species but possibly not all regions. The weirdness of the barrel cortex just makes its own second system more obvious.
A whisker only produces sensory inputs by producing forces at its base. That sensation is indirect, but multiple whiskers touch surfaces since there are a couple dozen, so it is like trying to recognize something by touching it with two sticks in the same hand. This analogy is not perfect, though. Whiskers move back and forth rhythmically at neural frequencies, such as five or twenty hertz. They might not be able to recognize objects during whisking because it is so fast, but animals can move their whiskers in other ways besides whisking or keep them still. There is a map of the whisked space in L2/3 during whisking, which makes sense if it recognizes objects because otherwise it would operate in terms of a constantly moving sensor. Another thing you’ll need to know which doesn’t fit anywhere is that each cortical column corresponds to a whisker.
I will draw heavily from the barrel cortex, but the details are not the point. I have no idea how this system works, just that it exists. Details are only evidence and part of the thinking process. Since the barrel cortex is weird, it may need some specializations to implement this system. Those specializations are valid evidence for this universal system, but I need to research this system elsewhere.
More relevant to a review of L6, the second system is a huge problem for getting the facts right. I was editing this post to add information from a batch of notes, but I ran into a problem. As a result, I am not sure whether dual-projecting CT cells are universal, and I cannot research things very well until I figure that out. I’ll have to research rodents and primates and not just primary regions until I run into information which solves this problem.
Here is the background. Dual-projecting CT cells are multi-columnar in nature, unlike primary CT cells. In rodent barrel cortex and visual cortex, they project to an elongated area of the primary thalamus, and they also project to L5a in the same and adjacent columns. So far, these aspects of dual-projecting CT cells seem universal in primary regions of rodents, although maybe not primates.
But barrel cortex is weird. It has septa, meaning space between columns (technically just in layer 4 but not here), which has different connections. Dual-projecting CT cells appear to project to septal L4 in barrel cortex, but any V1 equivalent of septa must be sparse or not targeted by dual-projecting CT cells because they do not project into layer 4. Also, septal L4 receives input from the higher order thalamic nucleus for whisking, so one article proposes it is actually another cortical region higher in the hierarchy. That does not seem to be the case, as we will see.
Barrel cortex is weird, and so is the associated higher order thalamic nucleus, POm. It is not exactly higher order. The posterior two-thirds is higher order, but it mainly projects to L1 of barrel cortex so it is not really relevant to layer 6. The anterior third is a convergence zone, neither primary nor higher order. Its cells are driven by the sensory input as well as by the barrel cortex. It projects to L1 like the nonconvergence zone, but it also projects to L5a, where slender tufted cells are located in rodents. There are multiple convergence zones in primates, but few studies acknowledge convergence zones. Convergence zones associated with primary cortex are clearly not universal. The septal parts of barrel cortex are probably driven by the convergence zone like the primary thalamus drives columns.
The convergence zone has axons in layer 5a, but that does not mean they drive slender tufted cell firing. They probably don’t because the effects of POm, although possibly just the nonconvergence zone, on the cortex are mainly metabotropic. However, POm and slender tufted cells have some things in common. Both often seem unresponsive in experiments and both are closely associated with behavior.
Those two associations are linked by behavioral gating. Slender tufted cells, albeit a small sample size, have elevated firing rates during whisking, and they project to the striatum, so they have a clear role in behavior. Meanwhile, the convergence zone (but likely not the higher order zone) of the POm is briefly inhibited by a subcortical structure called the zone incerta during sensory input. This inhibition causes the delayed responses in most experiments, and it might make the convergence zone entirely higher order because cortical responses are delayed compared to sensory inputs. However, signals from the motor cortex to the zone incerta inhibit these cells, allowing the convergence zone to respond immediately to the sensory input. That behavioral gating suggests the convergence zone causes slender tufted cells to fire more.
Whereas in upper L6 CT cells are denser in columns and CC cells are denser in septa, there is no difference in lower L6, suggesting that dual-projecting CT cells are part of a septa-linked system.
Behaviorally gated responses are a theme of this system, and so are elongated portions of the map of the sensor. Slender tufted cells have long-reaching axons in L2/3 and L6, unlike thick tufted L5 cells, and they probably drive very long receptive fields in L6 with elongated axon arbors. It just so happens that dual-projecting CT cells project to elongated portions of the thalamus. That does not mean they are driven by slender tufted cells, although that seems likely, but they are clearly part of the same system.
Corticocortical cells in L6 are strongly associated with behavior since they are probably mainly driven by the motor cortex, and they have transcolumnar axons, so they are probably related to this system in some way. They also might receive only motor signals about rotation, which could mesh with long receptive fields somehow.
Dual-projecting CT cells happen to have very long axon arbors in the primary thalamus, reminiscent of the long receptive fields in L6, so although they could easily lack those receptive fields, they are part of the same system and therefore closely associated with slender tufted cells. There is also a substantial group of CT cells which are unresponsive to any form of sensory stimulation. That is consistent with dual-projecting CT cells, which receive less thalamic input than primary CT cells, being involved in a behavior-linked system. Also, whereas in upper L6 CT cells are denser in columns and CC cells are denser in septa, there is no difference in lower L6, suggesting that dual-projecting CT cells are part of a septa-linked system but not primary CT cells. M1-projecting CC cells are denser in upper L6, so they could be septal.
Here’s the problem that started this line of thinking. The projections from dual-projecting CT cells to the primary thalamus in the whisking system form elongated axon arbors, but those arbors are not arbitrary. They’re all oriented along the vertical axis of the whisker pad (called whisker arcs), which is perpendicular to the axis along which most whisking motion occurs. That makes it seem like they serve no function. Maybe they’re just better that way for this sense. However, I doubt these axon arbors are all aligned the same way in the visual system. They have been proposed to be related to lines in the visual input, and it makes a lot of sense to modulate thalamic cells that correspond to the same feature the same way. A fixed line on the whisker pad could actually be an arbitrary line. It can only operate in terms of a whisker arc, but whiskers don’t just send a binary signal. They also detect the direction of whisker deflection, so the orientation of the feature can be arbitrary. If the elongated arbors in the visual system are arbitrary, then for whatever reason using multiple whiskers is probably required to unambiguously determine the orientation of the feature. However, that does not explain why it does not have arbors oriented in any direction on the whisker pad’s thalamic representation, since so far there seems no reason not to have those.
Consider what would happen during whisking if it had arbors oriented in any direction. The signals to the thalamus can last much longer than a whisking cycle. It can control the firing modes of cells for each whisker, but not for each place in the whisked space. There is not enough temporal precision to do so. If it tried to modulate cells oriented along the same row, it would interfere with other modulation. The whisker hits one feature and then another, but it has to have one firing mode for the whole cycle. This problem still exists for whiskers in the same arc, but it is more minor because whiskers along the same arc hit the surface around the same time.
What about when it isn’t whisking? With these arbitrarily oriented arbors, if it isn’t moving, it cannot operate in terms of other orientations of features. That means this system is only for behavior. When it is not behaving, the more normal system is sufficient. I do not know why that system lacks elongated axon arbors in the thalamus, although it might have those in visual cortex, probably on a smaller scale.
Layer 5 thick tufted cells project to the convergence zone. That might seem unnecessary since this system just needs to be gated by behavior. However, the sensory input during whisking is strongly tied to the behavior, so it makes sense to put it in context of something related to behavior. I do not think thick tufted cells just produce behavior but that’s a whole different topic.
This description of the second system includes specializations, so as described it is not in its pure form. V1 of the same species has dual-projecting CT cells (at least morphologically and in that they project to L5a), and V1 is the region where the elongated receptive fields were found, but there is no convergence zone for V1 and slender tufted cells do not receive higher order thalamic input there. V1 and barrel cortex L6 cells both receive motor copy signals, though. It is possible that combining results from these two regions (and in some cases elsewhere) could have caused problems.
Many questions about this system remain. What is the purpose of the elongated receptive fields and arbors? Does behavior just ungate this system, or does it have a crucial role in modalities with less dependence on behavior than whiskers? Why do the septal parts of barrel cortex have all the layers and are all of those divided into septal and normal? Does the septal system just solve problems unique to whisking? Are mechanisms in layer 6 for oscillations, brain states, attention, and so forth related to this? Attention kind of has to be related because it involves corticothalamic cells. Do duel-projecting CT cells only project to the convergence zone? What does the normal system do while the second system is active and can that help clarify the normal system? Is the second system universal? Are dual-projecting CT cells universal?
**Sources**
For Morphology and intracortical projections of functionally characterised neurones in the cat visual cortex, see http://www.neurosciences.us/courses/vision2/V1Circuits/gilbert79.pdf
For Burst and tonic firing in thalamic cells of unanesthetized, behaving monkeys, see http://shermanlab.uchicago.edu/files/RP126.pdf
References
-
Beierlein M, Connors BW. Short-term dynamics of thalamocortical and intracortical synapses onto layer 6 neurons in neocortex. J Neurophysiol. 2002;88(4):1924-32. https://www.physiology.org/doi/full/10.1152/jn.2002.88.4.1924
-
Zhou Y, Liu BH, Wu GK, et al. Preceding inhibition silences layer 6 neurons in auditory cortex. Neuron. 2010;65(5):706-17. https://www.cell.com/neuron/fulltext/S0896-6273(10)00138-8
-
Kinnischtzke AK, Fanselow EE, Simons DJ. Target-specific M1 inputs to infragranular S1 pyramidal neurons. J Neurophysiol. 2016;116(3):1261-74. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5018057/
-
Gilbert CD, Wiesel TN. Morphology and intracortical projections of functionally characterised neurones in the cat visual cortex. Nature. 1979;280(5718):120-5.
-
Vélez-fort M, Bracey EF, Keshavarzi S, et al. A Circuit for Integration of Head- and Visual-Motion Signals in Layer 6 of Mouse Primary Visual Cortex. Neuron. 2018;98(1):179-191.e6. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5896233/
-
Cotel F, Fletcher LN, Kalita-de croft S, Apergis-schoute J, Williams SR. Cell Class-Dependent Intracortical Connectivity and Output Dynamics of Layer 6 Projection Neurons of the Rat Primary Visual Cortex. Cereb Cortex. 2018;28(7):2340-2350. https://academic.oup.com/cercor/article/28/7/2340/3862197
-
Crandall SR, Patrick SL, Cruikshank SJ, Connors BW. Infrabarrels Are Layer 6 Circuit Modules in the Barrel Cortex that Link Long-Range Inputs and Outputs. Cell Rep. 2017;21(11):3065-3078. https://www.cell.com/cell-reports/fulltext/S2211-1247(17)31686-8?code=cell-site
-
Zarrinpar A, Callaway EM. Local connections to specific types of layer 6 neurons in the rat visual cortex. J Neurophysiol. 2006;95(3):1751-61. https://www.physiology.org/doi/full/10.1152/jn.00974.2005
-
Ledergerber D, Larkum ME. Properties of layer 6 pyramidal neuron apical dendrites. J Neurosci. 2010;30(39):13031-44. http://www.jneurosci.org/content/30/39/13031.long
-
Zhang ZW, Deschênes M. Intracortical axonal projections of lamina VI cells of the primary somatosensory cortex in the rat: a single-cell labeling study. J Neurosci. 1997;17(16):6365-79. http://www.jneurosci.org/content/17/16/6365.long
-
Groh A, Bokor H, Mease RA, et al. Convergence of cortical and sensory driver inputs on single thalamocortical cells. Cereb Cortex. 2014;24(12):3167-79. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4224239/
-
Briggs F. Organizing principles of cortical layer 6. Front Neural Circuits. 2010;4:3. https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/neuro.04.003.2010/full
-
Mercer A, West DC, Morris OT, Kirchhecker S, Kerkhoff JE, Thomson AM. Excitatory connections made by presynaptic cortico-cortical pyramidal cells in layer 6 of the neocortex. Cereb Cortex. 2005;15(10):1485-96. https://academic.oup.com/cercor/article/15/10/1485/396548
-
Thomson AM. Neocortical layer 6, a review. Front Neuroanat. 2010;4:13. https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnana.2010.00013/full
-
Marx M, Feldmeyer D. Morphology and physiology of excitatory neurons in layer 6b of the somatosensory rat barrel cortex. Cereb Cortex. 2013;23(12):2803-17. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3827708/
-
Kim J, Matney CJ, Blankenship A, Hestrin S, Brown SP. Layer 6 corticothalamic neurons activate a cortical output layer, layer 5a. J Neurosci. 2014;34(29):9656-64. http://www.jneurosci.org/content/34/29/9656.long
-
Landisman CE, Connors BW. VPM and PoM nuclei of the rat somatosensory thalamus: intrinsic neuronal properties and corticothalamic feedback. Cereb Cortex. 2007;17(12):2853-65. https://academic.oup.com/cercor/article/17/12/2853/384727
-
West DC, Mercer A, Kirchhecker S, Morris OT, Thomson AM. Layer 6 cortico-thalamic pyramidal cells preferentially innervate interneurons and generate facilitating EPSPs. Cereb Cortex. 2006;16(2):200-11. https://academic.oup.com/cercor/article/16/2/200/281524
-
Polsky A, Mel B, Schiller J. Encoding and decoding bursts by NMDA spikes in basal dendrites of layer 5 pyramidal neurons. J Neurosci. 2009;29(38):11891-903.
-
Viaene AN, Petrof I, Sherman SM. Properties of the thalamic projection from the posterior medial nucleus to primary and secondary somatosensory cortices in the mouse. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2011;108(44):18156-61. http://www.pnas.org/content/108/44/18156.long
-
Mease RA, Sumser A, Sakmann B, Groh A. Cortical Dependence of Whisker Responses in Posterior Medial Thalamus In Vivo. Cereb Cortex. 2016;26(8):3534-43. https://academic.oup.com/cercor/article/26/8/3534/2428841
-
Mease RA, Metz M, Groh A. Cortical Sensory Responses Are Enhanced by the Higher-Order Thalamus. Cell Rep. 2016;14(2):208-15. https://www.cell.com/cell-reports/fulltext/S2211-1247(15)01450-3
-
Castejon C, Barros-zulaica N, Nuñez A. Control of Somatosensory Cortical Processing by Thalamic Posterior Medial Nucleus: A New Role of Thalamus in Cortical Function. PLoS ONE. 2016;11(1):e0148169. https://journals.plos.org/plosone/article?id=10.1371/journal.pone.0148169
-
Wright N, Fox K. Origins of cortical layer V surround receptive fields in the rat barrel cortex. J Neurophysiol. 2010;103(2):709-24. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2822698/
-
Pluta SR, Lyall EH, Telian GI, Ryapolova-webb E, Adesnik H. Surround Integration Organizes a Spatial Map during Active Sensation. Neuron. 2017;94(6):1220-1233.e5. https://www.cell.com/neuron/fulltext/S0896-6273(17)30352-5
-
Kim J, Matney CJ, Blankenship A, Hestrin S, Brown SP. Layer 6 corticothalamic neurons activate a cortical output layer, layer 5a. J Neurosci. 2014;34(29):9656-64. http://www.jneurosci.org/content/34/29/9656.long
-
Hoerder-suabedissen A, Hayashi S, Upton L, et al. Subset of Cortical Layer 6b Neurons Selectively Innervates Higher Order Thalamic Nuclei in Mice. Cereb Cortex. 2018;28(5):1882-1897. https://academic.oup.com/cercor/article/28/5/1882/4897996
-
Swadlow HA. Efferent neurons and suspected interneurons in S-1 vibrissa cortex of the awake rabbit: receptive fields and axonal properties. J Neurophysiol. 1989;62(1):288-308. https://www.physiology.org/doi/abs/10.1152/jn.1989.62.1.288
-
Swadlow HA. Efferent neurons and suspected interneurons in second somatosensory cortex of the awake rabbit: receptive fields and axonal properties. J Neurophysiol. 1991;66(4):1392-409. https://www.physiology.org/doi/abs/10.1152/jn.1991.66.4.1392
-
Swadlow HA. Efferent neurons and suspected interneurons in motor cortex of the awake rabbit: axonal properties, sensory receptive fields, and subthreshold synaptic inputs. J Neurophysiol. 1994;71(2):437-53. https://www.physiology.org/doi/abs/10.1152/jn.1994.71.2.437
-
Swadlow HA, Hicks TP. Somatosensory cortical efferent neurons of the awake rabbit: latencies to activation via supra–and subthreshold receptive fields. J Neurophysiol. 1996;75(4):1753-9. https://www.physiology.org/doi/abs/10.1152/jn.1996.75.4.1753
-
Trageser JC, Keller A. Reducing the uncertainty: gating of peripheral inputs by zona incerta. J Neurosci. 2004;24(40):8911-5. http://www.jneurosci.org/content/24/40/8911.long
-
Thomson AM. Neocortical layer 6, a review. Front Neuroanat. 2010;4:13. https://academic.oup.com/cercor/article/28/5/1882/4897996
-
Marx M, Feldmeyer D. Morphology and physiology of excitatory neurons in layer 6b of the somatosensory rat barrel cortex. Cereb Cortex. 2013;23(12):2803-17. https://academic.oup.com/cercor/article/23/12/2803/463163
-
Kim J, Matney CJ, Blankenship A, Hestrin S, Brown SP. Layer 6 corticothalamic neurons activate a cortical output layer, layer 5a. J Neurosci. 2014;34(29):9656-64. https://europepmc.org/articles/pmc4099543
36 duplicate of 17
-
West DC, Mercer A, Kirchhecker S, Morris OT, Thomson AM. Layer 6 cortico-thalamic pyramidal cells preferentially innervate interneurons and generate facilitating EPSPs. Cereb Cortex. 2006;16(2):200-11. https://academic.oup.com/cercor/article/16/2/200/281524
-
Hoerder-suabedissen A, Hayashi S, Upton L, et al. Subset of Cortical Layer 6b Neurons Selectively Innervates Higher Order Thalamic Nuclei in Mice. Cereb Cortex. 2018;28(5):1882-1897. https://academic.oup.com/cercor/article/28/5/1882/4897996
-
Guo W, Clause AR, Barth-maron A, Polley DB. A Corticothalamic Circuit for Dynamic Switching between Feature Detection and Discrimination. Neuron. 2017;95(1):180-194.e5. https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0896627317304579
-
Ramcharan EJ, Gnadt JW, Sherman SM. Burst and tonic firing in thalamic cells of unanesthetized, behaving monkeys. Vis Neurosci. 2000;17(1):55-62. https://www.cambridge.org/core/journals/visual-neuroscience/article/burst-and-tonic-firing-in-thalamic-cells-of-unanesthetized-behaving-monkeys/C2E671E18B84923356CADF457C32F292
-
Connelly WM, Crunelli V, Errington AC. Variable Action Potential Backpropagation during Tonic Firing and Low-Threshold Spike Bursts in Thalamocortical But Not Thalamic Reticular Nucleus Neurons. J Neurosci. 2017;37(21):5319-5333. http://www.jneurosci.org/content/early/2017/04/27/JNEUROSCI.0015-17.2017
-
Whitmire CJ, Waiblinger C, Schwarz C, Stanley GB. Information Coding through Adaptive Gating of Synchronized Thalamic Bursting. Cell Rep. 2016;14(4):795-807. https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S2211124715015223
-
Bayer L, Serafin M, Eggermann E, et al. Exclusive postsynaptic action of hypocretin-orexin on sublayer 6b cortical neurons. J Neurosci. 2004;24(30):6760-4. http://www.jneurosci.org/content/24/30/6760.long
-
Maruoka H, Nakagawa N, Tsuruno S, Sakai S, Yoneda T, Hosoya T. Lattice system of functionally distinct cell types in the neocortex. Science. 2017;358(6363):610-615. http://science.sciencemag.org/content/358/6363/610
-
Oberlaender M, Boudewijns ZS, Kleele T, Mansvelder HD, Sakmann B, De kock CP. Three-dimensional axon morphologies of individual layer 5 neurons indicate cell type-specific intracortical pathways for whisker motion and touch. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2011;108(10):4188-93. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3053980/
-
Sillito AM, Jones HE, Gerstein GL, West DC. Feature-linked synchronization of thalamic relay cell firing induced by feedback from the visual cortex. Nature. 1994;369(6480):479-82. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/8202137?dopt=Abstract
-
Pluta SR, Lyall EH, Telian GI, Ryapolova-webb E, Adesnik H. Surround Integration Organizes a Spatial Map during Active Sensation. Neuron. 2017;94(6):1220-1233.e5. https://www.cell.com/neuron/fulltext/S0896-6273(17)30352-5